Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxi...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2014
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b2021-11-18T08:34:42ZSticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?1932-620310.1371/journal.pone.0086170https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b2014-01-01T00:00:00Zhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmid/24497938/?tool=EBIhttps://doaj.org/toc/1932-6203The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxiety and distress. To date, little is known about how the majority of people react to INMI, in particular whether evaluation of the experience impacts on chosen response behaviours or if attempts at controlling INMI are successful or not. The present study classified 1046 reports of how people react to INMI episodes. Two laboratories in Finland and the UK conducted an identical qualitative analysis protocol on reports of INMI reactions and derived visual descriptive models of the outcomes using grounded theory techniques. Combined analysis carried out across the two studies confirmed that many INMI episodes were considered neutral or pleasant, with passive acceptance and enjoyment being among the most popular response behaviours. A significant number of people, however, reported on attempts to cope with unwanted INMI. The most popular and effective behaviours in response to INMI were seeking out the tune in question, and musical or verbal distraction. The outcomes of this study contribute to our understanding of the aetiology of INMI, in particular within the framework of memory theory, and present testable hypotheses for future research on successful INMI coping strategies.Victoria J WilliamsonLassi A LiikkanenKelly JakubowskiLauren StewartPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleMedicineRScienceQENPLoS ONE, Vol 9, Iss 1, p e86170 (2014) |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Medicine R Science Q |
spellingShingle |
Medicine R Science Q Victoria J Williamson Lassi A Liikkanen Kelly Jakubowski Lauren Stewart Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
description |
The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxiety and distress. To date, little is known about how the majority of people react to INMI, in particular whether evaluation of the experience impacts on chosen response behaviours or if attempts at controlling INMI are successful or not. The present study classified 1046 reports of how people react to INMI episodes. Two laboratories in Finland and the UK conducted an identical qualitative analysis protocol on reports of INMI reactions and derived visual descriptive models of the outcomes using grounded theory techniques. Combined analysis carried out across the two studies confirmed that many INMI episodes were considered neutral or pleasant, with passive acceptance and enjoyment being among the most popular response behaviours. A significant number of people, however, reported on attempts to cope with unwanted INMI. The most popular and effective behaviours in response to INMI were seeking out the tune in question, and musical or verbal distraction. The outcomes of this study contribute to our understanding of the aetiology of INMI, in particular within the framework of memory theory, and present testable hypotheses for future research on successful INMI coping strategies. |
format |
article |
author |
Victoria J Williamson Lassi A Liikkanen Kelly Jakubowski Lauren Stewart |
author_facet |
Victoria J Williamson Lassi A Liikkanen Kelly Jakubowski Lauren Stewart |
author_sort |
Victoria J Williamson |
title |
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
title_short |
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
title_full |
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
title_fullStr |
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
title_full_unstemmed |
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
title_sort |
sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? |
publisher |
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
publishDate |
2014 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT victoriajwilliamson stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery AT lassialiikkanen stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery AT kellyjakubowski stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery AT laurenstewart stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery |
_version_ |
1718421601013202944 |