Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?

The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxi...

Descripción completa

Guardado en:
Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Victoria J Williamson, Lassi A Liikkanen, Kelly Jakubowski, Lauren Stewart
Formato: article
Lenguaje:EN
Publicado: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2014
Materias:
R
Q
Acceso en línea:https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b
Etiquetas: Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
id oai:doaj.org-article:01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b
record_format dspace
spelling oai:doaj.org-article:01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b2021-11-18T08:34:42ZSticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?1932-620310.1371/journal.pone.0086170https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b2014-01-01T00:00:00Zhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmid/24497938/?tool=EBIhttps://doaj.org/toc/1932-6203The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxiety and distress. To date, little is known about how the majority of people react to INMI, in particular whether evaluation of the experience impacts on chosen response behaviours or if attempts at controlling INMI are successful or not. The present study classified 1046 reports of how people react to INMI episodes. Two laboratories in Finland and the UK conducted an identical qualitative analysis protocol on reports of INMI reactions and derived visual descriptive models of the outcomes using grounded theory techniques. Combined analysis carried out across the two studies confirmed that many INMI episodes were considered neutral or pleasant, with passive acceptance and enjoyment being among the most popular response behaviours. A significant number of people, however, reported on attempts to cope with unwanted INMI. The most popular and effective behaviours in response to INMI were seeking out the tune in question, and musical or verbal distraction. The outcomes of this study contribute to our understanding of the aetiology of INMI, in particular within the framework of memory theory, and present testable hypotheses for future research on successful INMI coping strategies.Victoria J WilliamsonLassi A LiikkanenKelly JakubowskiLauren StewartPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleMedicineRScienceQENPLoS ONE, Vol 9, Iss 1, p e86170 (2014)
institution DOAJ
collection DOAJ
language EN
topic Medicine
R
Science
Q
spellingShingle Medicine
R
Science
Q
Victoria J Williamson
Lassi A Liikkanen
Kelly Jakubowski
Lauren Stewart
Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
description The vast majority of people experience involuntary musical imagery (INMI) or 'earworms'; perceptions of spontaneous, repetitive musical sound in the absence of an external source. The majority of INMI episodes are not bothersome, while some cause disruption ranging from distraction to anxiety and distress. To date, little is known about how the majority of people react to INMI, in particular whether evaluation of the experience impacts on chosen response behaviours or if attempts at controlling INMI are successful or not. The present study classified 1046 reports of how people react to INMI episodes. Two laboratories in Finland and the UK conducted an identical qualitative analysis protocol on reports of INMI reactions and derived visual descriptive models of the outcomes using grounded theory techniques. Combined analysis carried out across the two studies confirmed that many INMI episodes were considered neutral or pleasant, with passive acceptance and enjoyment being among the most popular response behaviours. A significant number of people, however, reported on attempts to cope with unwanted INMI. The most popular and effective behaviours in response to INMI were seeking out the tune in question, and musical or verbal distraction. The outcomes of this study contribute to our understanding of the aetiology of INMI, in particular within the framework of memory theory, and present testable hypotheses for future research on successful INMI coping strategies.
format article
author Victoria J Williamson
Lassi A Liikkanen
Kelly Jakubowski
Lauren Stewart
author_facet Victoria J Williamson
Lassi A Liikkanen
Kelly Jakubowski
Lauren Stewart
author_sort Victoria J Williamson
title Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
title_short Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
title_full Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
title_fullStr Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
title_full_unstemmed Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
title_sort sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
publishDate 2014
url https://doaj.org/article/01a8361ff0fd4ac4af8700a86f96f72b
work_keys_str_mv AT victoriajwilliamson stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery
AT lassialiikkanen stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery
AT kellyjakubowski stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery
AT laurenstewart stickytuneshowdopeoplereacttoinvoluntarymusicalimagery
_version_ 1718421601013202944