Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions

Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the...

Descripción completa

Guardado en:
Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: David M. Sobel, Jayd Blankenship
Formato: article
Lenguaje:EN
Publicado: Nature Portfolio 2021
Materias:
R
Q
Acceso en línea:https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4
Etiquetas: Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
id oai:doaj.org-article:120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4
record_format dspace
spelling oai:doaj.org-article:120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d42021-12-02T13:20:22ZPerspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions10.1038/s41598-021-84968-22045-2322https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d42021-03-01T00:00:00Zhttps://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84968-2https://doaj.org/toc/2045-2322Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the result of that distribution is a more equitable outcome. Four studies investigated children’s developing preferences for generating equitable distributions, focusing on the mechanisms for this development. Children were presented with two characters with different amount of resources, and then a third character who will distribute more resources to them. Three- to 8-year-olds were asked whether the third character should give an equal number of resources to the recipients, preserving the inequity, or an unequal number to them, creating an equitable outcome. Starting at age 7, children showed a preference for equitable distributions (Study 1, N = 144). Studies 2a (N = 72) and 2b (N = 48) suggest that this development is independent of children’s numerical competence. When asked to take the perspective of the recipient with fewer resources, 3- to 6-year-olds were more likely to make an equitable distribution (Study 3, N = 122). These data suggest that social perspective taking underlies children’s prosocial actions, and supports the hypothesis that their spontaneous capacity to take others’ perspectives develops during the early elementary-school years.David M. SobelJayd BlankenshipNature PortfolioarticleMedicineRScienceQENScientific Reports, Vol 11, Iss 1, Pp 1-8 (2021)
institution DOAJ
collection DOAJ
language EN
topic Medicine
R
Science
Q
spellingShingle Medicine
R
Science
Q
David M. Sobel
Jayd Blankenship
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
description Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the result of that distribution is a more equitable outcome. Four studies investigated children’s developing preferences for generating equitable distributions, focusing on the mechanisms for this development. Children were presented with two characters with different amount of resources, and then a third character who will distribute more resources to them. Three- to 8-year-olds were asked whether the third character should give an equal number of resources to the recipients, preserving the inequity, or an unequal number to them, creating an equitable outcome. Starting at age 7, children showed a preference for equitable distributions (Study 1, N = 144). Studies 2a (N = 72) and 2b (N = 48) suggest that this development is independent of children’s numerical competence. When asked to take the perspective of the recipient with fewer resources, 3- to 6-year-olds were more likely to make an equitable distribution (Study 3, N = 122). These data suggest that social perspective taking underlies children’s prosocial actions, and supports the hypothesis that their spontaneous capacity to take others’ perspectives develops during the early elementary-school years.
format article
author David M. Sobel
Jayd Blankenship
author_facet David M. Sobel
Jayd Blankenship
author_sort David M. Sobel
title Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
title_short Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
title_full Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
title_fullStr Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
title_full_unstemmed Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
title_sort perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
publisher Nature Portfolio
publishDate 2021
url https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4
work_keys_str_mv AT davidmsobel perspectivetakingasamechanismforchildrensdevelopingpreferencesforequitabledistributions
AT jaydblankenship perspectivetakingasamechanismforchildrensdevelopingpreferencesforequitabledistributions
_version_ 1718393194235822080