Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions
Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Nature Portfolio
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4 |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4 |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d42021-12-02T13:20:22ZPerspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions10.1038/s41598-021-84968-22045-2322https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d42021-03-01T00:00:00Zhttps://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84968-2https://doaj.org/toc/2045-2322Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the result of that distribution is a more equitable outcome. Four studies investigated children’s developing preferences for generating equitable distributions, focusing on the mechanisms for this development. Children were presented with two characters with different amount of resources, and then a third character who will distribute more resources to them. Three- to 8-year-olds were asked whether the third character should give an equal number of resources to the recipients, preserving the inequity, or an unequal number to them, creating an equitable outcome. Starting at age 7, children showed a preference for equitable distributions (Study 1, N = 144). Studies 2a (N = 72) and 2b (N = 48) suggest that this development is independent of children’s numerical competence. When asked to take the perspective of the recipient with fewer resources, 3- to 6-year-olds were more likely to make an equitable distribution (Study 3, N = 122). These data suggest that social perspective taking underlies children’s prosocial actions, and supports the hypothesis that their spontaneous capacity to take others’ perspectives develops during the early elementary-school years.David M. SobelJayd BlankenshipNature PortfolioarticleMedicineRScienceQENScientific Reports, Vol 11, Iss 1, Pp 1-8 (2021) |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Medicine R Science Q |
spellingShingle |
Medicine R Science Q David M. Sobel Jayd Blankenship Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
description |
Abstract How do young children develop a concept of equity? Infants prefer dividing resources equally and expect others to make such distributions. Between the ages of 3–8, children begin to exhibit preferences to avoid inequitable outcomes in their distributions, dividing resources unequally if the result of that distribution is a more equitable outcome. Four studies investigated children’s developing preferences for generating equitable distributions, focusing on the mechanisms for this development. Children were presented with two characters with different amount of resources, and then a third character who will distribute more resources to them. Three- to 8-year-olds were asked whether the third character should give an equal number of resources to the recipients, preserving the inequity, or an unequal number to them, creating an equitable outcome. Starting at age 7, children showed a preference for equitable distributions (Study 1, N = 144). Studies 2a (N = 72) and 2b (N = 48) suggest that this development is independent of children’s numerical competence. When asked to take the perspective of the recipient with fewer resources, 3- to 6-year-olds were more likely to make an equitable distribution (Study 3, N = 122). These data suggest that social perspective taking underlies children’s prosocial actions, and supports the hypothesis that their spontaneous capacity to take others’ perspectives develops during the early elementary-school years. |
format |
article |
author |
David M. Sobel Jayd Blankenship |
author_facet |
David M. Sobel Jayd Blankenship |
author_sort |
David M. Sobel |
title |
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
title_short |
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
title_full |
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
title_fullStr |
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
title_full_unstemmed |
Perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
title_sort |
perspective taking as a mechanism for children’s developing preferences for equitable distributions |
publisher |
Nature Portfolio |
publishDate |
2021 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/120ecd88bec2409b820f2327d61a85d4 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT davidmsobel perspectivetakingasamechanismforchildrensdevelopingpreferencesforequitabledistributions AT jaydblankenship perspectivetakingasamechanismforchildrensdevelopingpreferencesforequitabledistributions |
_version_ |
1718393194235822080 |