Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits
Abstract Although humans can direct their attention to visual targets with or without eye movements, it remains unclear how different brain mechanisms control visual attention and eye movements together and/or separately. Here, we measured MEG and fMRI data during covert/overt visual pursuit tasks a...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Nature Portfolio
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/4bbd5db87e604c43b3094a9460122c50 |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:4bbd5db87e604c43b3094a9460122c50 |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:4bbd5db87e604c43b3094a9460122c502021-12-02T16:14:55ZCommon cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits10.1038/s41598-021-93259-92045-2322https://doaj.org/article/4bbd5db87e604c43b3094a9460122c502021-07-01T00:00:00Zhttps://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-93259-9https://doaj.org/toc/2045-2322Abstract Although humans can direct their attention to visual targets with or without eye movements, it remains unclear how different brain mechanisms control visual attention and eye movements together and/or separately. Here, we measured MEG and fMRI data during covert/overt visual pursuit tasks and estimated cortical currents using our previously developed extra-dipole, hierarchical Bayesian method. Then, we predicted the time series of target positions and velocities from the estimated cortical currents of each task using a sparse machine-learning algorithm. The predicted target positions/velocities had high temporal correlations with actual visual target kinetics. Additionally, we investigated the generalization ability of predictive models among three conditions: control, covert, and overt pursuit tasks. When training and testing data were the same tasks, the largest reconstructed accuracies were overt, followed by covert and control, in that order. When training and testing data were selected from different tasks, accuracies were in reverse order. These results are well explained by the assumption that predictive models consist of combinations of three computational brain functions: visual information-processing, maintenance of attention, and eye-movement control. Our results indicate that separate subsets of neurons in the same cortical regions control visual attention and eye movements differently.Ken-ichi MorishigeNobuo HiroeMasa-aki SatoMitsuo KawatoNature PortfolioarticleMedicineRScienceQENScientific Reports, Vol 11, Iss 1, Pp 1-15 (2021) |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Medicine R Science Q |
spellingShingle |
Medicine R Science Q Ken-ichi Morishige Nobuo Hiroe Masa-aki Sato Mitsuo Kawato Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
description |
Abstract Although humans can direct their attention to visual targets with or without eye movements, it remains unclear how different brain mechanisms control visual attention and eye movements together and/or separately. Here, we measured MEG and fMRI data during covert/overt visual pursuit tasks and estimated cortical currents using our previously developed extra-dipole, hierarchical Bayesian method. Then, we predicted the time series of target positions and velocities from the estimated cortical currents of each task using a sparse machine-learning algorithm. The predicted target positions/velocities had high temporal correlations with actual visual target kinetics. Additionally, we investigated the generalization ability of predictive models among three conditions: control, covert, and overt pursuit tasks. When training and testing data were the same tasks, the largest reconstructed accuracies were overt, followed by covert and control, in that order. When training and testing data were selected from different tasks, accuracies were in reverse order. These results are well explained by the assumption that predictive models consist of combinations of three computational brain functions: visual information-processing, maintenance of attention, and eye-movement control. Our results indicate that separate subsets of neurons in the same cortical regions control visual attention and eye movements differently. |
format |
article |
author |
Ken-ichi Morishige Nobuo Hiroe Masa-aki Sato Mitsuo Kawato |
author_facet |
Ken-ichi Morishige Nobuo Hiroe Masa-aki Sato Mitsuo Kawato |
author_sort |
Ken-ichi Morishige |
title |
Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
title_short |
Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
title_full |
Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
title_fullStr |
Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
title_full_unstemmed |
Common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
title_sort |
common cortical areas have different neural mechanisms for covert and overt visual pursuits |
publisher |
Nature Portfolio |
publishDate |
2021 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/4bbd5db87e604c43b3094a9460122c50 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT kenichimorishige commoncorticalareashavedifferentneuralmechanismsforcovertandovertvisualpursuits AT nobuohiroe commoncorticalareashavedifferentneuralmechanismsforcovertandovertvisualpursuits AT masaakisato commoncorticalareashavedifferentneuralmechanismsforcovertandovertvisualpursuits AT mitsuokawato commoncorticalareashavedifferentneuralmechanismsforcovertandovertvisualpursuits |
_version_ |
1718384288425050112 |