Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making.
The frontal lobes subserve decision-making and executive control--that is, the selection and coordination of goal-directed behaviors. Current models of frontal executive function, however, do not explain human decision-making in everyday environments featuring uncertain, changing, and especially ope...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2012
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/757abb1a9490465f971c6c62eae01b1d |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:757abb1a9490465f971c6c62eae01b1d |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:757abb1a9490465f971c6c62eae01b1d2021-11-18T05:36:43ZReasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making.1544-91731545-788510.1371/journal.pbio.1001293https://doaj.org/article/757abb1a9490465f971c6c62eae01b1d2012-01-01T00:00:00Zhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmid/22479152/pdf/?tool=EBIhttps://doaj.org/toc/1544-9173https://doaj.org/toc/1545-7885The frontal lobes subserve decision-making and executive control--that is, the selection and coordination of goal-directed behaviors. Current models of frontal executive function, however, do not explain human decision-making in everyday environments featuring uncertain, changing, and especially open-ended situations. Here, we propose a computational model of human executive function that clarifies this issue. Using behavioral experiments, we show that unlike others, the proposed model predicts human decisions and their variations across individuals in naturalistic situations. The model reveals that for driving action, the human frontal function monitors up to three/four concurrent behavioral strategies and infers online their ability to predict action outcomes: whenever one appears more reliable than unreliable, this strategy is chosen to guide the selection and learning of actions that maximize rewards. Otherwise, a new behavioral strategy is tentatively formed, partly from those stored in long-term memory, then probed, and if competitive confirmed to subsequently drive action. Thus, the human executive function has a monitoring capacity limited to three or four behavioral strategies. This limitation is compensated by the binary structure of executive control that in ambiguous and unknown situations promotes the exploration and creation of new behavioral strategies. The results support a model of human frontal function that integrates reasoning, learning, and creative abilities in the service of decision-making and adaptive behavior.Anne CollinsEtienne KoechlinPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleBiology (General)QH301-705.5ENPLoS Biology, Vol 10, Iss 3, p e1001293 (2012) |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Biology (General) QH301-705.5 |
spellingShingle |
Biology (General) QH301-705.5 Anne Collins Etienne Koechlin Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
description |
The frontal lobes subserve decision-making and executive control--that is, the selection and coordination of goal-directed behaviors. Current models of frontal executive function, however, do not explain human decision-making in everyday environments featuring uncertain, changing, and especially open-ended situations. Here, we propose a computational model of human executive function that clarifies this issue. Using behavioral experiments, we show that unlike others, the proposed model predicts human decisions and their variations across individuals in naturalistic situations. The model reveals that for driving action, the human frontal function monitors up to three/four concurrent behavioral strategies and infers online their ability to predict action outcomes: whenever one appears more reliable than unreliable, this strategy is chosen to guide the selection and learning of actions that maximize rewards. Otherwise, a new behavioral strategy is tentatively formed, partly from those stored in long-term memory, then probed, and if competitive confirmed to subsequently drive action. Thus, the human executive function has a monitoring capacity limited to three or four behavioral strategies. This limitation is compensated by the binary structure of executive control that in ambiguous and unknown situations promotes the exploration and creation of new behavioral strategies. The results support a model of human frontal function that integrates reasoning, learning, and creative abilities in the service of decision-making and adaptive behavior. |
format |
article |
author |
Anne Collins Etienne Koechlin |
author_facet |
Anne Collins Etienne Koechlin |
author_sort |
Anne Collins |
title |
Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
title_short |
Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
title_full |
Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
title_fullStr |
Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
title_full_unstemmed |
Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
title_sort |
reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. |
publisher |
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
publishDate |
2012 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/757abb1a9490465f971c6c62eae01b1d |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT annecollins reasoninglearningandcreativityfrontallobefunctionandhumandecisionmaking AT etiennekoechlin reasoninglearningandcreativityfrontallobefunctionandhumandecisionmaking |
_version_ |
1718424856560664576 |