The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery
Over the past decade, biomarker discovery has become a key goal in psychiatry to aid in the more reliable diagnosis and prognosis of heterogeneous psychiatric conditions and the development of tailored therapies. Nevertheless, the prevailing statistical approach is still the mean group comparison be...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2021
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/79dd0471b99c4095b03db59b084ef2f2 |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:79dd0471b99c4095b03db59b084ef2f2 |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Biology (General) QH301-705.5 |
spellingShingle |
Biology (General) QH301-705.5 Eva Loth Jumana Ahmad Chris Chatham Beatriz López Ben Carter Daisy Crawley Bethany Oakley Hannah Hayward Jennifer Cooke Antonia San José Cáceres Danilo Bzdok Emily Jones Tony Charman Christian Beckmann Thomas Bourgeron Roberto Toro Jan Buitelaar Declan Murphy Guillaume Dumas The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
description |
Over the past decade, biomarker discovery has become a key goal in psychiatry to aid in the more reliable diagnosis and prognosis of heterogeneous psychiatric conditions and the development of tailored therapies. Nevertheless, the prevailing statistical approach is still the mean group comparison between “cases” and “controls,” which tends to ignore within-group variability. In this educational article, we used empirical data simulations to investigate how effect size, sample size, and the shape of distributions impact the interpretation of mean group differences for biomarker discovery. We then applied these statistical criteria to evaluate biomarker discovery in one area of psychiatric research—autism research. Across the most influential areas of autism research, effect size estimates ranged from small (d = 0.21, anatomical structure) to medium (d = 0.36 electrophysiology, d = 0.5, eye-tracking) to large (d = 1.1 theory of mind). We show that in normal distributions, this translates to approximately 45% to 63% of cases performing within 1 standard deviation (SD) of the typical range, i.e., they do not have a deficit/atypicality in a statistical sense. For a measure to have diagnostic utility as defined by 80% sensitivity and 80% specificity, Cohen’s d of 1.66 is required, with still 40% of cases falling within 1 SD. However, in both normal and nonnormal distributions, 1 (skewness) or 2 (platykurtic, bimodal) biologically plausible subgroups may exist despite small or even nonsignificant mean group differences. This conclusion drastically contrasts the way mean group differences are frequently reported. Over 95% of studies omitted the “on average” when summarising their findings in their abstracts (“autistic people have deficits in X”), which can be misleading as it implies that the group-level difference applies to all individuals in that group. We outline practical approaches and steps for researchers to explore mean group comparisons for the discovery of stratification biomarkers. Author summary Currently, a striking paradox is often found in neuropsychiatric research. On the one hand, most clinicians and researchers accept that many neuropsychiatric conditions involve tremendous individual variability. On the other hand, the prevailing statistical approach is still the mean group comparison between “cases” and “controls.” Statistically significant mean group differences tell us that a given characteristic in brain, behaviour, or genes is on average different between the 2 groups. Yet, they do not delineate variability within groups. Moreover, using autism research as an example, we show that in up to 95% of abstracts, when reporting or interpreting findings, researchers omit the “on average.” This can be misleading because it evokes the impression as though the group-level difference would generalise to all individuals with that condition. Here, we used simulations to show that the latter statement is only true at very large effect sizes. We demonstrate that across different areas of autism research, mean group differences with small to large effects indicate that approximately 45% to 68% [cases] do not have an atypicality on cognitive tests or brain structure. However, we also show that across normal and nonnormal distributions, subgroups may exist despite small or nonsignificant overall effects. We propose practical approaches and steps for researchers to use mean group comparisons as the starting point for the discovery of clinically relevant subgroups. |
format |
article |
author |
Eva Loth Jumana Ahmad Chris Chatham Beatriz López Ben Carter Daisy Crawley Bethany Oakley Hannah Hayward Jennifer Cooke Antonia San José Cáceres Danilo Bzdok Emily Jones Tony Charman Christian Beckmann Thomas Bourgeron Roberto Toro Jan Buitelaar Declan Murphy Guillaume Dumas |
author_facet |
Eva Loth Jumana Ahmad Chris Chatham Beatriz López Ben Carter Daisy Crawley Bethany Oakley Hannah Hayward Jennifer Cooke Antonia San José Cáceres Danilo Bzdok Emily Jones Tony Charman Christian Beckmann Thomas Bourgeron Roberto Toro Jan Buitelaar Declan Murphy Guillaume Dumas |
author_sort |
Eva Loth |
title |
The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
title_short |
The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
title_full |
The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
title_fullStr |
The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
title_full_unstemmed |
The meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
title_sort |
meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery |
publisher |
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
publishDate |
2021 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/79dd0471b99c4095b03db59b084ef2f2 |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT evaloth themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT jumanaahmad themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT chrischatham themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT beatrizlopez themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT bencarter themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT daisycrawley themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT bethanyoakley themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT hannahhayward themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT jennifercooke themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT antoniasanjosecaceres themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT danilobzdok themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT emilyjones themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT tonycharman themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT christianbeckmann themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT thomasbourgeron themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT robertotoro themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT janbuitelaar themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT declanmurphy themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT guillaumedumas themeaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT evaloth meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT jumanaahmad meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT chrischatham meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT beatrizlopez meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT bencarter meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT daisycrawley meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT bethanyoakley meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT hannahhayward meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT jennifercooke meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT antoniasanjosecaceres meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT danilobzdok meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT emilyjones meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT tonycharman meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT christianbeckmann meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT thomasbourgeron meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT robertotoro meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT janbuitelaar meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT declanmurphy meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery AT guillaumedumas meaningofsignificantmeangroupdifferencesforbiomarkerdiscovery |
_version_ |
1718414525568385024 |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:79dd0471b99c4095b03db59b084ef2f22021-11-25T05:42:06ZThe meaning of significant mean group differences for biomarker discovery1553-734X1553-7358https://doaj.org/article/79dd0471b99c4095b03db59b084ef2f22021-11-01T00:00:00Zhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8601419/?tool=EBIhttps://doaj.org/toc/1553-734Xhttps://doaj.org/toc/1553-7358Over the past decade, biomarker discovery has become a key goal in psychiatry to aid in the more reliable diagnosis and prognosis of heterogeneous psychiatric conditions and the development of tailored therapies. Nevertheless, the prevailing statistical approach is still the mean group comparison between “cases” and “controls,” which tends to ignore within-group variability. In this educational article, we used empirical data simulations to investigate how effect size, sample size, and the shape of distributions impact the interpretation of mean group differences for biomarker discovery. We then applied these statistical criteria to evaluate biomarker discovery in one area of psychiatric research—autism research. Across the most influential areas of autism research, effect size estimates ranged from small (d = 0.21, anatomical structure) to medium (d = 0.36 electrophysiology, d = 0.5, eye-tracking) to large (d = 1.1 theory of mind). We show that in normal distributions, this translates to approximately 45% to 63% of cases performing within 1 standard deviation (SD) of the typical range, i.e., they do not have a deficit/atypicality in a statistical sense. For a measure to have diagnostic utility as defined by 80% sensitivity and 80% specificity, Cohen’s d of 1.66 is required, with still 40% of cases falling within 1 SD. However, in both normal and nonnormal distributions, 1 (skewness) or 2 (platykurtic, bimodal) biologically plausible subgroups may exist despite small or even nonsignificant mean group differences. This conclusion drastically contrasts the way mean group differences are frequently reported. Over 95% of studies omitted the “on average” when summarising their findings in their abstracts (“autistic people have deficits in X”), which can be misleading as it implies that the group-level difference applies to all individuals in that group. We outline practical approaches and steps for researchers to explore mean group comparisons for the discovery of stratification biomarkers. Author summary Currently, a striking paradox is often found in neuropsychiatric research. On the one hand, most clinicians and researchers accept that many neuropsychiatric conditions involve tremendous individual variability. On the other hand, the prevailing statistical approach is still the mean group comparison between “cases” and “controls.” Statistically significant mean group differences tell us that a given characteristic in brain, behaviour, or genes is on average different between the 2 groups. Yet, they do not delineate variability within groups. Moreover, using autism research as an example, we show that in up to 95% of abstracts, when reporting or interpreting findings, researchers omit the “on average.” This can be misleading because it evokes the impression as though the group-level difference would generalise to all individuals with that condition. Here, we used simulations to show that the latter statement is only true at very large effect sizes. We demonstrate that across different areas of autism research, mean group differences with small to large effects indicate that approximately 45% to 68% [cases] do not have an atypicality on cognitive tests or brain structure. However, we also show that across normal and nonnormal distributions, subgroups may exist despite small or nonsignificant overall effects. We propose practical approaches and steps for researchers to use mean group comparisons as the starting point for the discovery of clinically relevant subgroups.Eva LothJumana AhmadChris ChathamBeatriz LópezBen CarterDaisy CrawleyBethany OakleyHannah HaywardJennifer CookeAntonia San José CáceresDanilo BzdokEmily JonesTony CharmanChristian BeckmannThomas BourgeronRoberto ToroJan BuitelaarDeclan MurphyGuillaume DumasPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleBiology (General)QH301-705.5ENPLoS Computational Biology, Vol 17, Iss 11 (2021) |