Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.

The closure of cognitive psychology labs around the world due to the COVID-19 pandemic has prevented in-person testing. This has caused a particular challenge for speech production researchers, as before the pandemic there were no studies demonstrating that reliable overt speech production data coul...

Descripción completa

Guardado en:
Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Amie Fairs, Kristof Strijkers
Formato: article
Lenguaje:EN
Publicado: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2021
Materias:
R
Q
Acceso en línea:https://doaj.org/article/8fd8360947944b3ba7d5b3e640d2f1d0
Etiquetas: Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
id oai:doaj.org-article:8fd8360947944b3ba7d5b3e640d2f1d0
record_format dspace
spelling oai:doaj.org-article:8fd8360947944b3ba7d5b3e640d2f1d02021-12-02T20:07:47ZCan we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.1932-620310.1371/journal.pone.0258908https://doaj.org/article/8fd8360947944b3ba7d5b3e640d2f1d02021-01-01T00:00:00Zhttps://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0258908https://doaj.org/toc/1932-6203The closure of cognitive psychology labs around the world due to the COVID-19 pandemic has prevented in-person testing. This has caused a particular challenge for speech production researchers, as before the pandemic there were no studies demonstrating that reliable overt speech production data could be collected via the internet. Here, we present evidence that both accurate and reliable overt articulation data can be collected from internet-based speech production experiments. We tested 100 participants in a picture naming paradigm, where we manipulated the word and phonotactic frequency of the picture names. We compared our results to a lab-based study conducted on different participants which used the same materials and design. We found a significant word frequency effect but no phonotactic frequency effect, fully replicating the lab-based results. Effect sizes were similar between experiments, but with significantly longer latencies in the internet-collected data. We found no evidence that internet upload or download speed affected either naming latencies or errors. In addition, we carried out a permutation-style analysis which recommends a minimum sample size of 40 participants for online production paradigms. In sum, our study demonstrates that internet-based testing of speech production is a feasible and promising endeavour, with less challenges than many researchers (anecdotally) assumed.Amie FairsKristof StrijkersPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleMedicineRScienceQENPLoS ONE, Vol 16, Iss 10, p e0258908 (2021)
institution DOAJ
collection DOAJ
language EN
topic Medicine
R
Science
Q
spellingShingle Medicine
R
Science
Q
Amie Fairs
Kristof Strijkers
Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
description The closure of cognitive psychology labs around the world due to the COVID-19 pandemic has prevented in-person testing. This has caused a particular challenge for speech production researchers, as before the pandemic there were no studies demonstrating that reliable overt speech production data could be collected via the internet. Here, we present evidence that both accurate and reliable overt articulation data can be collected from internet-based speech production experiments. We tested 100 participants in a picture naming paradigm, where we manipulated the word and phonotactic frequency of the picture names. We compared our results to a lab-based study conducted on different participants which used the same materials and design. We found a significant word frequency effect but no phonotactic frequency effect, fully replicating the lab-based results. Effect sizes were similar between experiments, but with significantly longer latencies in the internet-collected data. We found no evidence that internet upload or download speed affected either naming latencies or errors. In addition, we carried out a permutation-style analysis which recommends a minimum sample size of 40 participants for online production paradigms. In sum, our study demonstrates that internet-based testing of speech production is a feasible and promising endeavour, with less challenges than many researchers (anecdotally) assumed.
format article
author Amie Fairs
Kristof Strijkers
author_facet Amie Fairs
Kristof Strijkers
author_sort Amie Fairs
title Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
title_short Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
title_full Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
title_fullStr Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
title_full_unstemmed Can we use the internet to study speech production? Yes we can! Evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
title_sort can we use the internet to study speech production? yes we can! evidence contrasting online versus laboratory naming latencies and errors.
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
publishDate 2021
url https://doaj.org/article/8fd8360947944b3ba7d5b3e640d2f1d0
work_keys_str_mv AT amiefairs canweusetheinternettostudyspeechproductionyeswecanevidencecontrastingonlineversuslaboratorynaminglatenciesanderrors
AT kristofstrijkers canweusetheinternettostudyspeechproductionyeswecanevidencecontrastingonlineversuslaboratorynaminglatenciesanderrors
_version_ 1718375255166156800