Fitting and interpreting occupancy models.
We show that occupancy models are more difficult to fit than is generally appreciated because the estimating equations often have multiple solutions, including boundary estimates which produce fitted probabilities of zero or one. The estimates are unstable when the data are sparse, making them diffi...
Guardado en:
Autores principales: | , , |
---|---|
Formato: | article |
Lenguaje: | EN |
Publicado: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2013
|
Materias: | |
Acceso en línea: | https://doaj.org/article/c689ca523b9443fa87959a6593d99afe |
Etiquetas: |
Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
|
id |
oai:doaj.org-article:c689ca523b9443fa87959a6593d99afe |
---|---|
record_format |
dspace |
spelling |
oai:doaj.org-article:c689ca523b9443fa87959a6593d99afe2021-11-18T08:02:04ZFitting and interpreting occupancy models.1932-620310.1371/journal.pone.0052015https://doaj.org/article/c689ca523b9443fa87959a6593d99afe2013-01-01T00:00:00Zhttps://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmid/23326323/?tool=EBIhttps://doaj.org/toc/1932-6203We show that occupancy models are more difficult to fit than is generally appreciated because the estimating equations often have multiple solutions, including boundary estimates which produce fitted probabilities of zero or one. The estimates are unstable when the data are sparse, making them difficult to interpret, and, even in ideal situations, highly variable. As a consequence, making accurate inference is difficult. When abundance varies over sites (which is the general rule in ecology because we expect spatial variance in abundance) and detection depends on abundance, the standard analysis suffers bias (attenuation in detection, biased estimates of occupancy and potentially finding misleading relationships between occupancy and other covariates), asymmetric sampling distributions, and slow convergence of the sampling distributions to normality. The key result of this paper is that the biases are of similar magnitude to those obtained when we ignore non-detection entirely. The fact that abundance is subject to detection error and hence is not directly observable, means that we cannot tell when bias is present (or, equivalently, how large it is) and we cannot adjust for it. This implies that we cannot tell which fit is better: the fit from the occupancy model or the fit ignoring the possibility of detection error. Therefore trying to adjust occupancy models for non-detection can be as misleading as ignoring non-detection completely. Ignoring non-detection can actually be better than trying to adjust for it.Alan H WelshDavid B LindenmayerChristine F DonnellyPublic Library of Science (PLoS)articleMedicineRScienceQENPLoS ONE, Vol 8, Iss 1, p e52015 (2013) |
institution |
DOAJ |
collection |
DOAJ |
language |
EN |
topic |
Medicine R Science Q |
spellingShingle |
Medicine R Science Q Alan H Welsh David B Lindenmayer Christine F Donnelly Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
description |
We show that occupancy models are more difficult to fit than is generally appreciated because the estimating equations often have multiple solutions, including boundary estimates which produce fitted probabilities of zero or one. The estimates are unstable when the data are sparse, making them difficult to interpret, and, even in ideal situations, highly variable. As a consequence, making accurate inference is difficult. When abundance varies over sites (which is the general rule in ecology because we expect spatial variance in abundance) and detection depends on abundance, the standard analysis suffers bias (attenuation in detection, biased estimates of occupancy and potentially finding misleading relationships between occupancy and other covariates), asymmetric sampling distributions, and slow convergence of the sampling distributions to normality. The key result of this paper is that the biases are of similar magnitude to those obtained when we ignore non-detection entirely. The fact that abundance is subject to detection error and hence is not directly observable, means that we cannot tell when bias is present (or, equivalently, how large it is) and we cannot adjust for it. This implies that we cannot tell which fit is better: the fit from the occupancy model or the fit ignoring the possibility of detection error. Therefore trying to adjust occupancy models for non-detection can be as misleading as ignoring non-detection completely. Ignoring non-detection can actually be better than trying to adjust for it. |
format |
article |
author |
Alan H Welsh David B Lindenmayer Christine F Donnelly |
author_facet |
Alan H Welsh David B Lindenmayer Christine F Donnelly |
author_sort |
Alan H Welsh |
title |
Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
title_short |
Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
title_full |
Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
title_fullStr |
Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
title_full_unstemmed |
Fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
title_sort |
fitting and interpreting occupancy models. |
publisher |
Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
publishDate |
2013 |
url |
https://doaj.org/article/c689ca523b9443fa87959a6593d99afe |
work_keys_str_mv |
AT alanhwelsh fittingandinterpretingoccupancymodels AT davidblindenmayer fittingandinterpretingoccupancymodels AT christinefdonnelly fittingandinterpretingoccupancymodels |
_version_ |
1718422631878754304 |