Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms

Abstract Success at school determines future career opportunities. We described a time-of-day specific disparity in school performance between early and late chronotypes. Several studies showed that students with a late chronotype and short sleep duration obtain lower grades, suggesting that early s...

Descripción completa

Guardado en:
Detalles Bibliográficos
Autores principales: Giulia Zerbini, Vincent van der Vinne, Lana K. M. Otto, Thomas Kantermann, Wim P. Krijnen, Till Roenneberg, Martha Merrow
Formato: article
Lenguaje:EN
Publicado: Nature Portfolio 2017
Materias:
R
Q
Acceso en línea:https://doaj.org/article/cd0f27923187461b995b0265d996c081
Etiquetas: Agregar Etiqueta
Sin Etiquetas, Sea el primero en etiquetar este registro!
id oai:doaj.org-article:cd0f27923187461b995b0265d996c081
record_format dspace
spelling oai:doaj.org-article:cd0f27923187461b995b0265d996c0812021-12-02T15:06:13ZLower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms10.1038/s41598-017-04076-y2045-2322https://doaj.org/article/cd0f27923187461b995b0265d996c0812017-06-01T00:00:00Zhttps://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04076-yhttps://doaj.org/toc/2045-2322Abstract Success at school determines future career opportunities. We described a time-of-day specific disparity in school performance between early and late chronotypes. Several studies showed that students with a late chronotype and short sleep duration obtain lower grades, suggesting that early school starting times handicap their performance. How chronotype, sleep duration, and time of day impact school performance is not clear. At a Dutch high school, we collected 40,890 grades obtained in a variety of school subjects over an entire school year. We found that the strength of the effect of chronotype on grades was similar to that of absenteeism, and that late chronotypes were more often absent. The difference in grades between the earliest 20% and the latest 20% of chronotypes corresponds to a drop from the 55th to 43rd percentile of grades. In academic subjects using mainly fluid cognition (scientific subjects), the correlation with grades and chronotype was significant while subjects relying on crystallised intelligence (humanistic/linguistic) showed no correlation with chronotype. Based on these and previous results, we can expand our earlier findings concerning exam times: students with a late chronotype are at a disadvantage in exams on scientific subjects, and when they are examined early in the day.Giulia ZerbiniVincent van der VinneLana K. M. OttoThomas KantermannWim P. KrijnenTill RoennebergMartha MerrowNature PortfolioarticleMedicineRScienceQENScientific Reports, Vol 7, Iss 1, Pp 1-10 (2017)
institution DOAJ
collection DOAJ
language EN
topic Medicine
R
Science
Q
spellingShingle Medicine
R
Science
Q
Giulia Zerbini
Vincent van der Vinne
Lana K. M. Otto
Thomas Kantermann
Wim P. Krijnen
Till Roenneberg
Martha Merrow
Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
description Abstract Success at school determines future career opportunities. We described a time-of-day specific disparity in school performance between early and late chronotypes. Several studies showed that students with a late chronotype and short sleep duration obtain lower grades, suggesting that early school starting times handicap their performance. How chronotype, sleep duration, and time of day impact school performance is not clear. At a Dutch high school, we collected 40,890 grades obtained in a variety of school subjects over an entire school year. We found that the strength of the effect of chronotype on grades was similar to that of absenteeism, and that late chronotypes were more often absent. The difference in grades between the earliest 20% and the latest 20% of chronotypes corresponds to a drop from the 55th to 43rd percentile of grades. In academic subjects using mainly fluid cognition (scientific subjects), the correlation with grades and chronotype was significant while subjects relying on crystallised intelligence (humanistic/linguistic) showed no correlation with chronotype. Based on these and previous results, we can expand our earlier findings concerning exam times: students with a late chronotype are at a disadvantage in exams on scientific subjects, and when they are examined early in the day.
format article
author Giulia Zerbini
Vincent van der Vinne
Lana K. M. Otto
Thomas Kantermann
Wim P. Krijnen
Till Roenneberg
Martha Merrow
author_facet Giulia Zerbini
Vincent van der Vinne
Lana K. M. Otto
Thomas Kantermann
Wim P. Krijnen
Till Roenneberg
Martha Merrow
author_sort Giulia Zerbini
title Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
title_short Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
title_full Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
title_fullStr Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
title_full_unstemmed Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
title_sort lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
publisher Nature Portfolio
publishDate 2017
url https://doaj.org/article/cd0f27923187461b995b0265d996c081
work_keys_str_mv AT giuliazerbini lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT vincentvandervinne lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT lanakmotto lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT thomaskantermann lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT wimpkrijnen lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT tillroenneberg lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
AT marthamerrow lowerschoolperformanceinlatechronotypesunderlyingfactorsandmechanisms
_version_ 1718388542260903936